The research ties specific patterns of the hormone cortisol,
released into the bloodstream in times of stress, with cognitive
abilities for children in poverty.
Insensitive parenting and family instability were the strongest
predictors of children's cortisol profiles, even stronger than other
factors like interpartner violence, said lead author Jennifer H.
Suor, a doctoral student in clinical psychology at the University of
Rochester.
“Extensive research has shown that many low-income children face a
variety of social stressors, such as chaotic and unpredictable
family environments and problematic parenting practices as economic
hardship is known to place considerable burden on the family
system,” Suor told Reuters Health by email.
The researchers studied 201 pairs of low-income mothers and their
two-year old children recruited through community assistance
programs in Rochester, New York. Almost all were receiving public
assistance and living below the federal poverty line.
More than half of the mothers identified themselves as black.
At the first visit, researchers observed the mother-child pairs
playing with toys and puzzles for 10 minutes and rated the mother’s
“emotional availability.” They looked for her awareness of the
child’s needs, moods, interests, and capabilities.
The researchers also interviewed the mothers about the frequency,
nature, course, and aftermath of parental or partner conflicts the
children had witnessed. Mothers also filled out family instability
questionnaires assessing any caregiver changes, residential changes,
caregiver intimate relationship changes, job or income loss, and
family member deaths which had occurred over the previous three
years.
Every year for three years, the researchers collected two saliva
samples from the children to monitor their cortisol levels.
At the end of the study, when the children were four years old,
their cognitive abilities were measured with a preschool and primary
scale test.
Levels of cortisol - high, moderate or low - in saliva remained
generally stable over the three year period, as reported in the
journal Child Development. More than a third of the children had low
levels of cortisol, and 30 percent had high levels.
Those with higher and lower cortisol levels tended to have poorer
mental functioning at age four. Those with the higher cortisol
pattern generally had more insensitive interactions with their
mothers during the observation period at age two.
Children with the moderate cortisol profile had experienced less
family adversity at age two and had the highest cognitive
performance at age four.
The hormone cortisol can cross from blood into the brain. Too much
or too little is thought to have a negative impact on brain
structures and other neurobiological processes, but the exact
mechanism is still unknown, Suor said.
[to top of second column] |
“The study was what we call, a ‘correlational design,’ so we cannot
infer causality,” she said.
Other environmental and biological factors may contribute to lower
cognitive functioning, she said.
“Poverty undeniably is a context that is not good for children and
families, and there are many potential sources of stress for low
income children,” said J.J. Cutuli, who studies resilience in
development at Rutgers University-Camden in New Jersey.
“At the same time, stress in children’s lives tends to go along with
stress in parents’ lives which might interfere with the types of
stimulation they provide or other parenting behaviors that help
young children learn to regulate their emotions,” Cutuli told
Reuters Health by email.
Poverty tends to limit opportunities for early education, he noted.
But income does not determine someone’s level of cognitive
functioning, he said.
“Poverty is undeniably correlated with lower levels of cognitive
functioning when you compare groups of low income children to higher
income children on average,” Cutuli said. “But many low income
children outperform many higher income children.”
Certain protective factors help some children stay resilient in
poverty, including positive relationships with adults, especially
parents, extended family members, teachers, and coaches, he said.
“Our sample was only composed of low-income families so we are
unsure of how cognitive functioning is differentially impacted
across varying levels of socio-economic status,” Suor said. “I think
we as a society should be worried about this and consider developing
or increasing accessibility of community-based programs that might
better support families living in poverty, who are faced with a
variety of social stressors, which might have a spillover effect
(on) children's cortisol and in turn, their cognitive outcomes.”
Supporting families living in poverty through services and programs
that might alleviate stress might in turn help to support healthy
cortisol functioning and better cognitive and mental health outcomes
in children, Suor said.
SOURCE: http://bit.ly/1LhFiF7 Child Development, online June 17,
2015.
[© 2015 Thomson Reuters. All rights
reserved.] Copyright 2015 Reuters. All rights reserved. This material may not be published,
broadcast, rewritten or redistributed. |