Researchers conducted an experiment in six Utah elementary schools,
offering some kids cash and others praise for eating these healthy
foods, while a third group of children got no incentives at all.
Fruit and vegetable consumption was significantly higher in the
group of students who got cash rewards.
Cash prizes may have helped convince kids to overcome their fear of
trying new things, said study co-author Greg Madden, a psychology
professor at Utah State University in Logan.
“The rewards can be used to encourage children to repeatedly try
fruits and vegetables, and there is some evidence to suggest that
repeatedly tasting novel foods increases their acceptability,”
Madden said by email.
Madden and colleagues tested the power of cash prizes at schools
participating in a program called Food Dudes, which uses role
modeling, repeat tasting and rewards for eating fruits and
vegetables.
The study included 882 students offered prizes for eating more
fruits and veggies, 640 children who were praised by teachers for
doing this and a third group of 770 kids who didn’t get any
recognition at all based on their food choices.
At the start of the study, researchers photographed students’ lunch
trays before and after meals for three days to assess what they were
given to eat and how much they actually consumed. During this stage,
they didn’t specify how many fruits or vegetables the children were
served.
After that, the researchers provided all students – including kids
who brought lunch from home – the same fruits and vegetables, with
younger children receiving quarter-cup servings and older students
getting one-third cup portions. Again, researchers took before and
after pictures to see how much the children ate.
Then, researchers added rewards or praise to the children assigned
to these groups over the next 16 days. To encourage tasting,
students initially received bite-size portions of new fruits and
vegetables, and portions were gradually increased.
All of the kids got hand stamps if they accepted the fruits and
vegetables, and another hand stamp if they ate each serving. After
lunch, teachers gave cash to some students with four stamps and
praised others. A control group of students received the same
portions of fruits and vegetables but didn’t get cash or praise.
Compared with students who didn’t get any incentives, students who
got prizes increased their fruit and vegetable consumption by 0.32
cups, while kids who received praise consumed 0.21 cups more.
[to top of second column] |
Over about three months, the kids self-reported their fruit and
vegetable consumption, and teachers tracked their progress on wall
charts in the classroom, while continuing to offer prizes or praise
to students in those study groups.
Then, the rewards stopped. Researchers checked back with the kids
six months later to see what they put on their lunch trays and what
they ate and found the students in the prize group still ate
significantly more fruits and vegetables than their peers.
It’s possible that the delay between fruit and vegetable consumption
and the delivery of any reward might have diluted the effectiveness
of the incentives, the authors acknowledge in the Journal of the
Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. It’s also possible that the
students exaggerated how much they ate when they reported their own
eating habits.
Even when the rewards work in school, however, this is only one
piece of the puzzle, noted Samantha Heller, a nutritionist at New
York University's Center for Musculoskeletal Care and Sports
Performance who wasn't involved in the study.
“Rewarding good behavior such as healthy eating is a step in the
right direction,” Heller said by email.
But kids don’t eat every meal at school.
“The children still need strategies for managing healthy eating
choices outside the cafeteria and when the rewards stop,” Heller
added.
SOURCE: http://bit.ly/1LZYm8X Journal of the Academy of Nutrition
and Dietetics, online August 18, 2015.
[© 2015 Thomson Reuters. All rights
reserved.] Copyright 2015 Reuters. All rights reserved. This material may not be published,
broadcast, rewritten or redistributed.
|