"We found that the most relevant component of children's language
exposure is not the sheer number of words they hear, but the amount
of back-and-forth adult-child conversation they experience," said
lead study author Rachel Romeo of Boston Children's Hospital and the
Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
"These 'conversational turns' are strongly related to the physical
strength of white-matter connections between the two key language
regions in the left hemisphere of the brain," Romeo said by email.
"Most importantly, this relationship between conversational turns
and brain structure held independent of family socioeconomic status,
indicating the importance of turns across all sociodemographic
backgrounds."
Much of the advice parents get on the importance of talking to young
kids dates to a landmark study in the early 1990s that found by the
time children enter elementary school, kids from low-income families
have typically been exposed to 30 million fewer words than kids from
more affluent households. Since then, researchers and educators have
been examining how increased language exposure in early childhood
might help close income-based achievement gaps in school-age
children.
For the current study, researchers examined data from recordings of
all conversations between 40 children and their parents over two
consecutive weekend days. Children ranged in age from 4 to 6 years
old, and their parents came from diverse income and education
levels.
From the recordings, researchers calculated how many words children
heard adults speak and how many words the kids spoke. They also
looked for conversational turns by measuring how many exchanges
occurred with no more than five seconds passing between something
said by the child and a response from the adult.
Then, researchers looked at brain scans of the children and found
greater conversational turn-taking associated with stronger
connections between two brain regions, known as Wernicke's area and
Broca's area, that are central to the comprehension and production
of speech.
Families' socioeconomic backgrounds did not appear to influence the
results, researchers report in The Journal of Neuroscience.
Beyond its small size, another limitation of the study is that many
parents of girls failed to complete the home recordings, leaving 27
boys and only 13 girls in the analysis. It's also possible that
parents' conversations with their kids on recording days differed
from what they might sound like at other times.
[to top of second column] |
Even so, the results add to a large and growing body of evidence
suggesting that efforts to increase conversations in low-income
households might help reduce the chance that these children will
underperform relative to more affluent kids in school, the authors
conclude.
"Previous studies have demonstrated that the quality of early
language interactions have a significant impact on later language
and cognitive skills," said Natalie Brito a developmental
psychologist at New York University in New York City who wasn't
involved in the study.
"But this is the first study to find associations connecting home
language exposure, brain structure, and language skills," Brito said
by email.
While children in the current study did have better scores on tests
of verbal skills when parents had higher income and education
levels, conversation turns still independently influenced these
scores, noted Dr. Caroline Kistin, a pediatrics researcher at Boston
University School of Medicine and Boston Medical Center.
In the current study, higher parental income and education levels
were associated with higher verbal scores. But when the authors
statistically controlled for those factors in their analysis,
conversational turns were still associated with higher verbal
scores, indicating that the differences were not due solely to
socioeconomic status.
"Back-and-forth adult-child conversation likely improves language
development for all children," Kistin, who wasn't involved in the
study, said by email. "Organizations that work with young children
should recognize the importance of the caregiver-child bond and
support families in caring for their children and forming supportive
relationships that have been shown to positively influence child
development."
SOURCE: https://bit.ly/2OA9DDf The Journal of Neuroscience, online
August 13, 2018.
[© 2018 Thomson Reuters. All rights
reserved.] Copyright 2018 Reuters. All rights reserved. This material may not be published,
broadcast, rewritten or redistributed.
Thompson Reuters is solely responsible for this content. |