Study of polyglots offers insight on brain's language processing
Send a link to a friend
[March 12, 2024]
By Will Dunham
WASHINGTON (Reuters) - While most people speak only one language or
perhaps two, some are proficient in many. These people are called
polyglots. And they are helping to provide insight into how the brain
deals with language, the principal method of human communication.
In a new study involving a group of polyglots, the brain activity of the
participants was monitored using a method called functional magnetic
resonance imaging as they listened to passages read in various
languages.
With one intriguing exception, activity increased in the areas of the
cerebral cortex involved in the brain's language-processing network when
these polyglots - who spoke between five and 54 languages - heard
languages in which they were the most proficient compared to ones of
lesser or no proficiency.
"We think this is because when you process a language that you know
well, you can engage the full suite of linguistic operations - the
operations that the language system in your brain supports," said
Massachusetts Institute of Technology neuroscientist Evelina Fedorenko,
a member of MIT's McGovern Institute for Brain Research and senior
author of the study published on Monday in the journal Cerebral Cortex.
"You can access all the word meanings from memory, you can build phrases
and clauses out of the individual words, and you can access complex,
sentence-level meanings," Fedorenko added.
But an exception caught the attention of the researchers. In many of the
participants, listening to their native language elicited a lesser brain
response compared to hearing other languages they knew - on average down
about 25%. And in some of the polyglots, listening to their native
language activated only a part of the brain's language network, not the
whole thing.
"Polyglots become experts in their native language from the point of
view of efficiency of neural processes that are required to process it.
Therefore, the language network in the brain does not activate as much
when they do native versus non-native language processing," said
neuroscientist and study co-lead author Olessia Jouravlev of Carleton
University in Canada.
"One's native language may hold a privileged status, at least in this
population," Fedorenko added, referring to the study's polyglot
participants.
The brain's language network involves a few areas situated in its
frontal and temporal lobes.
[to top of second column]
|
Japanese Yuuka Hasumi, 17, and Yuho Wakamatsu, 15, attend a Korean
language class in Seoul, South Korea, March 12, 2019. REUTERS/Kim
Hong-Ji/File Photo
"The language network supports comprehension and production across
modalities - spoken, written, signed, etc. - and helps us encode our
thoughts into word sequences and decode others' thoughts from their
utterances," Fedorenko said.
Study co-lead author Saima Malik-Moraleda, a doctoral student at the
Harvard/MIT Program in Speech and Hearing Bioscience and Technology,
said the findings suggest that the distillation of meaning governs
brain response to language.
"The more meaning you can extract from the language input you are
receiving, the greater the response in language regions - except for
the native language, presumably because the speaker is more
efficient in extracting meaning from the linguistic input,"
Malik-Moraleda said.
The 34 study participants, 20 men and 14 women, ranged in age from
19 to 71. Twenty-one were native English speakers, with the rest
native speakers of French, Russian, Spanish, Dutch, German,
Hungarian and Mandarin Chinese.
Their brain activity was monitored when they listened to recordings
of passages in eight languages: their native language, three others
in which they were highly proficient, moderately proficient and
minimally proficient, and then four they did not know. Half heard
recordings of Lewis Carroll's "Alice in Wonderland." The other half
heard recordings of biblical stories.
The lesser brain response to hearing one's native language was most
pronounced among the study participants who heard the biblical
stories - linguistically simpler, according to Fedorenko, than
Carroll's writing.
"A lot of work in language research," Fedorenko said, "has focused
on individuals with linguistic difficulties - developmental or
acquired. But we can also learn a lot about cognitive and neural
infrastructure of some function by looking at individuals who are
'experts' in that function. Polyglots are one kind of language
'experts.'"
(Reporting by Will Dunham, Editing by Rosalba O'Brien)
[© 2024 Thomson Reuters. All rights reserved.]This material
may not be published, broadcast, rewritten or redistributed.
Thompson Reuters is solely responsible for this content. |